Thursday, September 8, 2016

Why Colleges And Universities Should Not Disinvite Speakers

Since 2000, more than 140 people who have been invited to speak on American college and university campuses have been “disinvited” before they could give their talks, usually after objections from students. It is easy to find news accounts of these events, or non-events: this handy database details (as of this writing) 342 successful and unsuccessful disinvitation campaigns. One of the most prominent recent examples may be the New York University administration's decision in September 2016 to cancel a lecture by the Nobel prizewinning biologist James Watson, on strategies for curing cancer, six days before it was scheduled to happen, because of student complaints about statements Watson had made on other topics in the past. Later in the same academic year, Watson was also disinvited from giving a talk on the same topic at the University of Illinois.

Notably, these events were not reported in any mainstream media outlets, which mostly seem to regard suppressing lectures as normal business in academia today. Indeed, campus groups are less likely to invite controversial speakers in the first place, given how likely it is that such invitations will meet with opposition and possible cancellation. It is much harder to find stories about academic leaders who clearly rejected demands for disinvitation and clearly explained why. If I were a college president, and a campaign to disinvite a speaker arose on my campus, here is the letter I would write.

To The Campus Community:

Recently, an organization on our campus announced that a particular person has been invited to speak here. Many students, some faculty, and a few alumni of our institution have publicly objected to the invitation of this speaker. Some have demanded that his invitation be rescinded, so that he will not be able to use our “platform,” and the imprimatur of our college will not be attached to the controversial things he was expected to say—or to anything he has said or written in the past. It has been alleged that the speaker will make some students feel uncomfortable or unsafe, that his beliefs are repugnant, and that his ideas are not rational or grounded in solid evidence.

I have heard these demands, and listened to the arguments of their supporters. I am writing to say that I do not agree with them, to announce that the speaker’s talk will go forward as planned, and to explain why.

First, let’s be clear that this is not a matter of “freedom of speech” in the legal sense. The First Amendment to the U.S. Constitution, as interpreted by the courts, says that governments may not abridge freedom of speech, but it puts no restrictions on private institutions like our college. Disinviting someone is unprofessional and rude, but we have the legal right to disinvite—or to not invite in the first place—anyone we please.

Some will say that if I do not disinvite this speaker, I am therefore supporting him. This is not the case. There is a clear logical distinction between endorsing a person’s claims and beliefs, and giving him an opportunity to express those claims and beliefs. There are many speakers who have come to our campus with whom I disagree, but I did not block them. We do not ban books from our library or websites from our network, so we do not ban speakers from our grounds. Constitutional law does allow for some non-commercial speech, such as explicit incitements to violence or disorder, to be punished by the state. And I would certainly agree to interrupt or block a speech that was directly threatening anyone’s immediate physical safety. But beyond that, it is not my place to decide who is and is not permitted to speak here.

In fact, no one’s personal preferences should have anything to do with this question. To see why, we must consider what our college, or any college, is here for. An institution of higher education is organized around the concept of learning. Learning is why we are all part of this community. Students are here to learn about the arts, sciences, and other disciplines they pursue. Professors are here to learn as well: to learn entirely new things about the natural, social, and humanistic worlds, and to learn how to teach more effectively. The staff and administration are here to make these endeavors possible.

Learning doesn’t just mean going to class, doing homework, and taking exams. If a group of students or faculty members are so interested in hearing, debating, and engaging with the ideas of a person from outside our community that they decide to invite him here and organize and attend an event, I cannot rebuke them. In fact, I congratulate them, for they are engaged in an act of learning that goes beyond what is strictly required of them. They are spending their personal time and energy furthering the central purpose of our institution. Even though I may not like all the speakers they are selecting, I still love the fact that they are bothering to select speakers at all.

The reputation of our college and the value of the degrees we confer will not be affected by the speakers we host, but it will suffer if we acquire a reputation for stifling unpopular views. A college does not need to “manage” its “brand,” and it should not act like a for-profit company in this respect. All colleges stand for excellence in scholarship; that is the only brand that matters, and disinviting speakers and suppressing thoughts will only cheapen it.

Even speakers who espouse ideas you find dangerous and are sure you would never accept—like a creationist, a 9/11 “truther,” a genocide denier, or someone who argues that “rape culture” is a liberal myth—may be worth hearing. If you find out what they claim for evidence, and the kinds of words, phrases, and arguments they use, you can better rebut them yourself—whether you are reasoning with other people, or questioning your own beliefs. Listen to the other side's case in order to strengthen your own. In other words, know thy enemy.

To those who say the speaker may make them feel unsafe, I must point out that higher education is not designed to make people safe. Instead, it is our society’s designated “safe space” for disruptive intellectual activity. It’s a space that has been created and set apart specifically for the incubation of knowledge, by both students and faculty. Ideas that may seem dangerous or repugnant can be expressed here—even if nowhere else—so that they can be analyzed, discussed, and understood as dispassionately as possible. Many of humanity’s greatest achievements originated as ideas that were suppressed from the public sphere. Some, like the theory of evolution by natural selection, equal rights for women and minorities, trade unions, democracy, and ironically even the right to free speech and expression, are still seen as dangerous decades and centuries later.

If you are against this speaker coming here, please also consider this: Some members of our community—some of your friends and colleagues—do want him to visit. By asking me to disinvite him, you are implicitly claiming that your concerns and preferences are more important than those of the people who invited him. Are you really sure that you are so right and they are so wrong? Psychologists have found that people tend to be overconfident in their beliefs, and poor at taking the perspective of others. That might be the case here.

A decision by me to bar this speaker would have far-reaching negative repercussions. It will make everyone in our community think twice before they stage a provocative event or invite a controversial speaker. Canceling this invitation will not only prevent this person from talking; it will reduce the expression of views like his in the future, and probably chill speech by anyone who could be regarded as controversial. And it will set a precedent that future leaders in higher education may point to if they feel pressured to do the same. All of this would be antithetical to our common purpose—and our institution's social function—of learning and discovery.

Note that it’s especially important for us to be open to viewpoints not already well-represented among our faculty. The professors here are a diverse group, but many studies have shown that professors tend to be more politically left-wing than the population at large. Even the most conscientious instructor may inadvertently slant his teaching and assignments towards his own political viewpoint. Of course, this applies more in the social sciences and humanities than in math or physics, but it does happen. Giving campus organizations wide latitude to invite the speakers they wish helps to increase the range of thoughts that are aired and discussed here.

If you feel that this speaker’s talk might upset you, I offer this advice: Go. Yes, go to the talk, listen to it, record it—if the speaker and hosts give permission—and think about it. Expose yourself to ideas that trouble you, because avoiding sources of anxiety is not the best way to cope with them. When you encounter troubling ideas on our campus, try to desensitize yourself to emotional reactions by keeping in mind that ideas themselves cannot hurt you.

And please do not try to distract, interrupt, or shout down the speaker. "Deplatforming" a speaker risks making him into a "free speech martyr" who will attract more followers because he is seen as a teller of truths so dangerous that his opponents try to ban them. Just ignore him, rather than spark an explosion that might win him new fans or deepen the ardor of the ones he already has. It is a natural impulse for us to suppress speech that we don’t like, just as it is natural for us to retaliate against or outlaw behavior we don’t like. That’s why we have laws to protect unpopular speech and institutions to foster and study it. You can take this golden opportunity to train yourself to respond to speech that upsets you by listening to it, analyzing it, looking up its sources, developing reasoned counterarguments to it, and considering why people agree with it and whether it might not be as contemptible as you have been told. These are the intellectual skills that all members of our community are committed to building.

In fact, if you’re already committed to everything this speaker is against, then you should definitely listen to him. John Stuart Mill wrote, “He who knows only his own side of the case, knows little of that.” When you never encounter people who vigorously argue for positions you don’t agree with, you may come to believe that those arguments don’t have merit, don’t deserve to be heard, or don’t even exist. The argument you imagine your opponents making is probably weaker and easier to dismiss than the argument they would actually make if they had the chance.

Of course, you don’t have to listen to speakers you disagree with. That’s the beauty of our system: We are all committed to the broad goal of learning, but we are never forced to attend to people we can’t stand. If you want to protest this speaker, do so peacefully, outside the venue, and do not block anyone from attending. Hand out fliers or arrange for other speakers to present counterarguments or different ideas. As Justice Louis Brandeis said, “If there be time to expose through discussion the falsehood and fallacies, to avert the evil by the process of education, the remedy to be applied is more speech, not enforced silence.” And if just being in the speaker’s presence will cause too much discomfort, you may avail yourself of the truly safe space of your dorm room or apartment, or the company of other like-minded students.

Please be careful, though, about making a habit of avoiding or trying to suppress uncomfortable ideas. In the wider world there are no spaces where you can be safe from the thoughts in other people’s heads, so if people are stereotyping you or otherwise judging you unfairly, nothing that restricts speech here on our campus will solve that problem. Holding negative thoughts and uttering negative speech are a part of human nature that our college does not exist to protect you from. On the contrary, we exist to arm you with the intellectual tools to understand, analyze, and dispute incorrect ideas. Shutting off those ideas does nothing to inoculate you against them, and may ironically make you even more vulnerable in the future.

The same is true of our college—of our community—as a whole. Once an organization stops challenging itself with ideas, old or new, it becomes intellectually flaccid and surrenders any claim to scholarly excellence. Valuing comfort and community over openness to ideas is perfectly fine for many organizations. Religions, charitable causes, and political parties are free and sometimes even wise to exclude ideas and people that they disagree with. But the essential common value in a university is free inquiry for the purpose of learning, and by joining the university we have all sacrificed our right to be safe from ideas we disagree with. Community is important here, but openness is fundamental. It’s for that simple reason that I will not disinvite any speaker who has been legitimately invited to talk to us.

Ruth Simmons, the former president of Brown University, told the graduating Smith College class of 2014, “The collision of views and ideologies is in the DNA of the academic enterprise. We don't need any collision-avoidance technology here.” I could not agree more. Therefore, as the leader of this community of scholars, of our academic enterprise, I would be doing the opposite of my duty were I to force silence on this or any other speaker. I hereby decline the requests to disinvite him. And I encourage all campus groups and organizations to invite the speakers they want to hear, knowing that I will respect and support your efforts to learn and engage with their ideas.

Sincerely,

Your College President


NOTE: This was originally published on 8 September 2016 and was revised and updated on 4 June 2017 and 7 April 2018. If you represent a mainstream online or print publication and would like to publish a version of this essay, please contact me.

Sunday, January 24, 2016

Confusion About Correlation and Causation ... in a Research Methods Textbook?!

Every so often, textbook publishers send me free copies of their books. Usually these are books for courses I teach, but sometimes they aren't. This week I received a copy of Discovering the Scientist Within: Research Methods in Psychology from Worth Publishers, a new title in its first edition. I don't teach a research methods course, but I flipped through the table of contents anyhow, and I noticed an entry called "Research Spotlight: The Upside to Video-Game Play" on page 31. Since the question of how playing video games might affect cognition and behavior is a controversial one, I was curious to see what the authors had to say about it in a research methods context. Unfortunately, their discussion has some problems.

First, they claim that "there are some real advantages to playing video games," citing a finding that "more time spent playing video games coincided with greater visual-spatial skills." Stating that playing video games has advantages is a statement of causality. If people who played video games just happened to have greater visual-spatial skills (maybe because their visual-spatial skills were greater than those of non-gamers to start, or because their visual-spatial skills were improving faster than those of non-gamers), there would be no "advantage" to the game-playing. The abstract of the underlying paper by Jackson et al. (2011) makes no mention of random assignment of participants to different amounts of video game play, so there's no justification for inferring causality. (Additionally, it notes that the video-game players had lower GPAs.)

Second, they say that "it just so happens that surgeons benefit from video-game playing as well," citing a study by Rosser et al. (2007) that found that "surgeons who played video games for more than 3 hours a week made 37% fewer errors and were 27% faster in laparoscopic surgery and suturing drills compared to surgeons who never played video games." This is followed by speculation as to the mechanism by which game playing could cause these differences. However, the evidence of causation here is even weaker than for the study of children cited above. It's not even a longitudinal study—it's just a cross-sectional finding of an association between video game play and performance on (computerized) tests of surgical skill. Again, one need only read as far as the Rosser et al. abstract to find the statement "Video game skill correlates with laparoscopic surgical skills." There is no evidence of causality, but the textbook authors have said that surgeons "benefit" from playing video games.

Finally, the caption below the stock photo reinforces the thrust of the boxed text by asking "If video games can make you a better surgeon, what other ares of your life could playing video games improve?" As they say in courtroom dramas, "Objection! Assumes facts not in evidence."

Sure, this is a run-of-the-mill mistake that laypeople make all the time: Confusing evidence of correlation (video game playing co-occurring with increased spatial skill or surgical proficiency) for evidence of causation (playing video games making your spatial skills and surgical proficiency better than they were before). But this is a textbook on research methods in psychology. If the authors of such books have the proverbial "one job to do," it is teaching their readers what conclusions can be drawn from what kinds of evidence. That's what education in research methods is all about: learning to design research studies that have the power to permit certain inferences, and learning which inferences can and cannot logically follow from which designs. You can think of analogies to other fields—a nutrition book reversing the properties of carbohydrates and fat? An algebra textbook getting the quadratic formula wrong? A history book that confuses the Declaration of Independence for the Constitution? Correlation versus causation is not a nuance or side issue; it's at the heart of the behavioral science enterprise.

The authors of Discovering the Scientist Within must understand the distinction between correlation and causation, and I am sure they can generate the plausible alternative (non-causal) explanations for these video game results that I mention above. I know this because on page 30, in the paragraph immediately before the "Research Spotlight" box, they write, "often there is not a set direction of how one thing influences another ... News coverage, such as in cases of school shootings, often portrays playing video games as the cause of aggressive behavior. Yet it is equally likely that aggressive individuals gravitate toward violent video games" [emphasis added].

The fact that mistakes like this can turn up in a book meant to educate its readers to avoid them is remarkable, and I think it goes to show just how confounding sound causal inference can be for the human mind. As Daniel Simons and I argued in The Invisible Gorilla, human beings are susceptible to an "illusion of cause" that leads us to jump to particular causal conclusions in all kinds of situations where the evidence we have doesn't logically justify them—indeed, where other explanations are equally or even more likely, and where the assumption of causality can get us into big trouble. The ease with which we can generate mechanisms to explain a particular causal inference can contribute to the illusion. For example, being aware of the "neural plasticity" concept could make it seem more likely that intensive cognitive work (e.g., video-gaming) might "train" some more fundamental underlying cognitive capacity (e.g., spatial skill) or transfer to some other practical task (e.g., surgical proficiency). None of us, not even psychology professors who write textbooks on research methods, are immune to these fundamental thinking pitfalls.

Hopefully the second edition of Discovering the Scientist Within will correct these correlation/causation errors, as well as any other issues that may lurk in the text. My quick flip-through picked up one more passage the authors might want to think about rewriting:
... the author Malcolm Gladwell, a self-described "cover band for psychology," is known for his ability to summarize and synthesize psychological findings so that the general public can benefit from the exciting advances in knowledge that psychological researchers have made.
Accompanying this sentence on page 41 is a photo of Gladwell's book Outliers. As many readers of this blog will know, I don't agree that the general public is benefitting from Malcolm Gladwell's writing, precisely because he doesn't summarize and synthesize as well as people think he does. Since correlation, causation, and statistical thinking are among the things Gladwell has difficulty with, it doesn't seem like a research methods textbook should be endorsing his work.